Conservation (psychology)
Conservation refers to a logical thinking ability which, according to the psychologist Jean Piaget, is not present in children during the preoperational stage of their development at ages 2–7, but develops in the concrete operational stage at ages 7–11.[1][2] Conservation refers to the ability to determine that a certain quantity will remain the same despite adjustment of the container, shape, or apparent size.
Tasks
Conservation tasks test a child’s ability to see that some properties are conserved or invariant after an object undergoes physical transformation. The following tasks also explain the different types of conservation. Piaget proposed that children's inability to conserve is due to weakness in the way children think during the preoperational stage (ages 2–6). This stage of cognitive development is characterized by children focusing on a single, salient dimension of height or length, while ignoring other important dimensions about a situation.[2] Children during this stage also tend to focus on the static characteristics of objects, instead of focusing on when objects undergo changes, which is a critical element of the following tasks.[2]
Liquid
Piaget’s most famous task, the conservation of liquid task involved showing a child two beakers A1 and A2, both of which were identical and which contained the same amount of colored (typically blue) liquid. Then liquid from the second glass A2 was poured into two taller, thinner glasses B1 and B2. The child was then asked whether there was still the same amount of liquid in both the new glasses (B1 and B2) as in the first glass A2. A child who cannot conserve would answer "no, there is more in the tall thin glasses," while a child who can conserve would answer "yes, there is still the same amount." Piaget also replicated this task with beads in glasses, and found again that some children were able to conserve while others were not.[1][3]
Number
For conservation of number, the task designed to test children involves a set of several marbles. These marbles are placed into two parallel lines that are the same length. Then the researcher spreads out the marbles in one line, longer than the other. Lastly the researcher asks "Is there the same number or a different number in both lines of marbles?" A child that cannot conserve, will answer that there are more marbles in the longer line, while a child that can conserve will recognize that there are the same amounts of marbles in both lines.[2]
Solid quantity
For conservation of solid quantity, the task designed to assess children involves two lumps of clay. The researcher first rolls the two lumps into the same shape. Then they researcher stretches out one of the balls into a long oblong shape. The researcher asks the child whether the two clay shapes have the same amount of clay or a different amount. A child that cannot conserve will answer that the shapes have different amounts of clay—that the oblong shape has more. The child than can conserve will understand that they still both have the same amount of clay.[2] Conservation of solid quantity is harder for children to learn than conservation of liquid and occurs later.[4]
Weight
For conservation of weight, the task involves two lumps of clay and a balance. The experimenter places two equal balls of clay onto either side of a balance and shows that the weights are the same. The experimenter then molds one ball of clay into an oblong shape, and asks the child if the two pieces of clay will still weight the same amount. A child that cannot conserve will answer that they now weight a different amount, while a child that can conserve will recognize that shape does not affect weight and respond that they weigh the same amount.[3]
Age
The ages at which children are able to complete conservation tasks varies; individual differences can cause some children to develop the ability later or earlier than others. Also age can vary across different countries (see conservation across cultures). However most children are not able to perform the conservation of number task correctly from ages 4–5, and most children develop the ability from ages 6–8. Conservation of mass and length occurs around age 7, conservation of weight around age 9, and conservation of volume around 11.[3][5]
Piaget's studies of conservation led him to observe the stages which children pass through when gaining the ability to conserve. In the first stage, children do not yet have the ability to conserve. During the conservation of liquid task, children will respond that a liquid in a tall glass always has more liquid than that of a short glass; they cannot discern height from amount. In the second stage, children expand their judgments in the conservation of liquid task to also include width as a reason; they may answers that a shorter, stouter glass has more liquid than a tall, skinny glass. In the third stage, children have gained the ability to conserve, and recognize that height and width do not affect amount.[3] Conservers believe more firmly in their answers on conservation tasks, when paired with nonconservers as partners, and they are able to offer multiple explanations and are more likely to manipulate the task materials to prove their point than nonconservers.[6]
In many cases, training tasks are successful in teaching non-conserving children to correctly complete conservation tasks.[5] Children as young as four years of age can be trained to conserve using operant training; this involves repeating conservation tasks and reinforcing correct responses while correcting incorrect responses.[7] The effects of training on one conservation task (such as conservation of liquid) often transfer to other conservation tasks.[5]
In connection with education
Research shows that conserving children demonstrate greater fluency in separately timed addition and subtraction problems than non-conserving children.[8] This research highlights the importance of logical-reversible thought, an element necessary to conserve, as being a critical component to a child's ability to perform inverse mathematical problems fluently (5+2=7; 7-5=2). For nonconserving children, research indicates that teachers should engage with children and ask them questions often about objects in their surrounds to encourage the development of more logical thinking.[9]
Across cultures
Most studies indicate that conservation occurs in a similar stages across cultures, but that there are differences in the rate at which conservation (and other cognitive abilities are acquired) across cultures. For example one study examining U.S. and Zambian female adolescents found no difference in between their ability to correctly answer conservation of weight tasks.[10] Another study looked at children from many countries (Australia, Netherlands, England, New Zealand, Poland, and Uganda) and tested for the ages at which conservation appears. They found that the rate at which children acquired conservation varied slightly in different countries, but that the age trends for when conservation develops are similar across borders despite cultural upbringing.[11] A review of cross-cultural studies looking at Piagetian tasks supported this finding, and found that while the general stages of cognitive development outlined by Piaget do occur across cultures, the rate of development is not consistent across cultures and sometimes the final stage of concrete operational thinking is not reached by all cultures due to environmental factors.[12]
A great deal of care is needed in cross-cultural examinations of conservation in order to avoid biasing results. For example, one study that tested North African Wolof adolescents found they were not able to conserve in a conservation of liquid task.[13] However another study suggests that their interpretation of the experimenter’s purpose may have conflicted with giving straightforward answers to the standard Piagetian questions because - except in school interrogation - Wolof people seldom ask questions to which they already know the answers.[14] When presented with the task as language-learning questions about the meaning of quantity terms such as "more" and "the same", the responses reflected understanding of conservation.[15]
Criticism of research methods
The conservation tasks (and hence Piaget's theory) have been criticized on a number of fronts in regards to research methods. Many studies have looked at variations of the conservation tasks and how these variations affect children's responses. For example, studies show that children need to be assessed both verbally and non-verbally, as assessing them solely in a verbal manner can lead test results indicating that a proportion of the children are unable to conserve, while in actuality some children are only able to answer conservation tasks correctly in a non-verbal manner.[16]
Research has suggested that asking the same question twice leads young children to change their answer as they assume that they are being asked again because they got it wrong first time around.[17] The importance of context was also emphasized by researchers who altered the task so that a 'naughty teddy' changed the array rather than an experimenter themselves. This seemed to give children a clear reason for the second question being asked, and four-year-old children were able to demonstrate knowledge of the conservation of matter much earlier than Piaget's reported 7- to 11-year-old threshold for concrete operations.[18]
In non-human primates
Research has also examined whether non-human primates are able to conserve. Chimpanzees are able to make judgements on whether two quantities of liquid are the same or different, and they are able to conserve correctly when liquids are transformed based on inferences. They are also able to correctly conserve for solid quantities, but they are not able to complete conservation of number tasks.[19][20] Orangutans are able to differentiate between same and different quantities of liquid but they are only able to pseudoconserve, similar to children in the second stage of conservation development, and ultimately fail to correctly complete conservation of liquid tasks (see Age of conservation ability).[21]
References
- 1 2 Piaget, J. (1965). The child's conception of number. New York: W. Norton Company & Inc.
- 1 2 3 4 5 Siegler, R., DeLoache, J., & Eisenberg., N. (2003). How children develop. New York: Worth Publishers.
- 1 2 3 4 Ginsburg, H. & Opper, S. (1969). Piaget's theory of intellectual development. Eaglewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, Inc.
- ↑ Twidle, J. "Is the concept of conservation of volume in solids really more difficult than for liquids, or is the way we test giving us an unfair comparison? (2006)". Educational research. 48 (1): 93–100. doi:10.1080/00131880500498511.
- 1 2 3 Hetherington, E. M. & Parke, R. D. (1975). Child Psychology: A contemporary viewpoint. New York: McGraw-Hill.
- ↑ Miller, S.A. & Brownell, C. A. (1977) Peers, persuasion, and Piaget: Dyadic interaction between conservers and nonconservers. In contemporary readings in child psychology. Eds. Hetherington, E. M. & Parke, R. D. 171-176. New York: McGraw-Hill.
- ↑ Bucher, B.; Schneider, R. E. (1973). "Acquisition and generalization of conservation by pre-schoolers, using operant training". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 16: 187–204. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(73)90160-4.
- ↑ Wubbena, Zane (2013). "Mathematical fluency as a function of conservation ability in young children". Learning and Individual Differences. 26: 153–155. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2013.01.013.
- ↑ Ojose, B. Applying Piaget’s Theory of cognitive development to mathematics instruction. The Mathematics Educator, 18 (1), 26-30.
- ↑ Brekke, B. W.; Williams, J. D.; Brekke, A. G. (1977). "Cross-cultural investigation of conservation of weight in Zambian adolescents and young adults". Perceptual and Motor Skills. 44: 417–418. doi:10.2466/pms.1977.44.2.417.
- ↑ Goldschmid, M. L.; Bentler, P. M.; Debus, R. L.; Rawlinson, R.; Kohnstamm, D.; Modgil, S.; Nicholls, J. F.; Reykowski, J.; Strupczewska, B; Warren, N. (1973). "A cross-cultural investigation of conservation". Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 4: 75–88. doi:10.1177/002202217300400106.
- ↑ Dasen, P. R. (1972). "Cross-cultural Piagetian research: a summary". Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 3 (1): 23–39. doi:10.1177/002202217200300102.
- ↑ Greenfield, P.M. (1966). On culture and conservation. In J.S. Bruner, R.R. Olver, & P.M. Greenfield (Eds.), Studies in cognitive growth. New York: Wiley.
- ↑ Irvine, J.T. (1978). "Wolof "magical thinking": Culture and conservation revisited". Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 9: 300–310. doi:10.1177/002202217893003.
- ↑ Rogoff, B. (2003). The Cultural Nature of Human Development. New York: Oxford University Press.
- ↑ Wheldall, K.; Benner, H. "Conservation without conversation revisited: a replication an elaboration of the Wheldall-Pobrca findings on the nonverbal assessment of conservation of liquid quantity (1993)". Education Psychology. 13 (1): 49–58. doi:10.1080/0144341930130106.
- ↑ Rose, S.A.; Blank, M. (1974). "The potency of context in children's cognition: An illustration through conservation". Child Development. 45 (2): 499–502. doi:10.2307/1127977.
- ↑ McGarrigle, J.; Donaldson, M. (1974). "Conservation accidents". Cognition. 3: 341–350. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(74)90003-1.
- ↑ Woodruff, G.; Premack, D.; Kennel, K (1978). "Conservation of liquid and solid quantity by the chimpanzee". Science. 202 (4371): 991–994. doi:10.1126/science.202.4371.991.
- ↑ Muncer, S. J. (1983). "Conservations" with a chimpanzee". Developmental Psychology. 16 (1): 1–11. doi:10.1002/dev.420160102.
- ↑ Call, J.; Rochat, P. (1996). "Liquid conservation in orangutans pongo pygmaeus) and humans (homo sapiens): Individual differences and perceptual strategies". Comparative Psychology. 110 (3): 219–232. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.110.3.219.