Father Tongue hypothesis

The Father Tongue hypothesis proposes that humans tend to speak their fathers' language. It is based on the discovery, in 1997, of a closer correlation between language and Y chromosomal variation than between language and mitochondrial DNA variation. The initial work was performed on African and European samples by a team of population geneticists led by Laurent Excoffier.[1][2] On the basis of these and similar findings by other geneticists, the hypothesis was elaborated by historical linguist Georg van Driem in 2010 that the teaching by a mother of her spouse’s tongue to her children is a mechanism by which language has preferentially been spread over time.[3]

Focusing on prehistoric language shift in already settled areas, examples worldwide[4] show that as little as 10-20% of prehistoric male immigration can (but need not) cause a language switch, indicating an elite imposition such as may have happened with the appearance of the first farmers or metalworkers in the neolithic, bronze and iron ages.

Early autosomal research

Before the discovery of mtDNA variation and Y chromosomal variation in the 1980s and 1990s, respectively,[5][6] it was not possible to distinguish male from female effects in population genetics. Instead, researchers had to rely on autosomal variation, starting with the first population genetic study using blood groups by Ludwik Hirszfeld in 1919.[7] Later other genetic polymorphisms were used, for example polymorphisms of proteins of the blood plasma, polymorphisms of human lymphocyte antigens or polymorphisms of immunoglobulins.[8] On this basis, correlations between languages and genetic variation occasionally were proposed,[9][10] but sex-specific questions could not be addressed until the 1990s, when both mtDNA and Y chromosomal variation in humans became available for study.

Origin of the hypothesis

The Y chromosome follows a patrilineal line of inheritance, meaning it is only passed on among males, from father to son. Mitochondrial DNA on the other hand follows a matrilineal line of inheritance, meaning it is only passed on from the mother to her children and from her daughters to their children. In 1997 Laurent Excoffier, his student Estella Poloni and his team reported that they had found a strong correlation between the Y-Chromosome sequence P49a,f/Taql variation and linguistics, while not being able to find such a correspondence for the mtDNA variation. Poloni et al. proposed the possible consequences of such a correlation, i.e. the father tongue hypothesis:

"As a consequence, the female-specific diversity of our genome would fit less well with geography and linguistics than would our male-specific component. [...] If that were to prove to be the case, then the common belief that we speak our mother's tongue should be revised in favor of the concept of a ‘father's tongue’."[1]

Estella Poloni also presented the father tongue hypothesis at an international conference in Paris in April 2000.[11]

On the basis of this population genetic work, historical linguist George van Driem elaborated the father tongue hypothesis in his ethnolinguistic publications and in population genetic publications which he has co-authored.[12] At the Indo-Pacific Prehistory Association conference in Taipei in 2002 he proposed that

"a mother teaching her children their father’s tongue has been a recurrent, ubiquitous and prevalent pattern throughout linguistic history, […] some of the mechanisms of language change over time are likely to be inherent to the dynamics of this pathway of transmission. Such correlations are observed worldwide."[13]

Discovery of Y markers for languages

The next development was the discovery of specific Y markers linked to a language.[14] [15][16] These Y variants do not cause language change, but happened to be carried by the historic or prehistoric male speakers spreading the language. These language-specific Y markers create correlations such as those observed by Poloni et al. 1997, and furthermore allow the geographic extent, the time depth and the male immigration level underlying an unrecorded (prehistoric) language change to be determined. Thus, as little as 10-20% of prehistoric male immigration can (but need not) cause a language switch, indicating an elite imposition such as may have happened with the appearance of the first farmers or metalworkers in the neolithic, bronze and iron ages.[4]

Examples of father tongues

There are several salient examples where the prehistoric diffusion of a language family correlates strongly with the diffusion of Y chromosomal haplogroups.[4]

Implications of the father tongue hypothesis

The father tongue hypothesis has far reaching implications for several processes in linguistics such as language change, language acquisition and sociolinguistics.

The historical linguist George van Driem interpreted the correlation of Y chromosomal haplogroups and language families as indicating that the spread of language families was often mediated by male-biased migration, whether these intrusions were martial or something less glamourous. He conjectured that the majority of language communities spoke father tongues rather than mother tongues.[17]

Focusing on prehistoric language shift in already settled areas, examples worldwide[4] show that as little as 10-20% of prehistoric male immigration can (but need not) cause a language switch, indicating an elite imposition such as may have happened with the appearance of the first farmers or metalworkers in the neolithic, bronze and Iron Ages.

The father tongue hypothesis has implications for linguists’ understanding of language change. It must be assumed that the dynamics of language change whereby mothers pass on the language of their spouses to their offspring differ from the dynamics of language change in a monolingual community and even from the dynamics of change in a bilingual community where mothers pass on their own language to their children.[21] As a consequence, such dynamics can introduce a discontinuity with the past. For example, it has been observed that Michif, genetically an Algonquian language was relexified by Métis women with French, the language of their husbands, and so the genetic affinity of Michif has come to be almost unidentifiable.[22][23][24] If the process of relexification went beyond the possibility of linguistic reconstruction, the dynamics of such a process may preclude the true linguistic heritance of a community.[21]

The father tongue hypothesis also has implications for language acquisition, as the hypothesis suggests an evolutionary explanation for why females may be better in some aspects of language performance and acquisition.[25][26][27][28][29][30][31][32][33]

Exceptions to the father tongue hypothesis

Genetics does not determine the language spoken by a human being, and the link between Y chromosomal haplogroups and linguistic affinities is an observed correlation and not a causal link. Therefore, it must be assumed that, while father tongues predominate, exceptions to father tongues exist in the world. Two very well known exceptions are the Balti in northern Pakistan and Hungarians. The mtDNA haplogroups most frequent among Balti are the same as those of the neighbouring Tibetan communities, whereas the Y chromosomal haplogroups most frequent in Balti males appear to have entered Baltistan from the west with the introduction of Islam. The Balti speak among the most conservative dialects of Tibetan.[12] The language of the Baltis corresponds with the mtDNA and not with the Y chromosome and is in effect a salient example of a mother tongue.[17] The other well known exception is Hungarian. The N1c haplogroup of the Y chromosome, distinguished by Tat-C deletion is found at a high frequency throughout Uralic language communities, but it is virtually missing in Hungarian males. Therefore, while the intrusion of the Magyars into what is today Hungary is historically attested and has left clear linguistic evidence, genetically the Magyar intrusion has left no salient genetic traces. Instead, from a genetic point of view, Hungarians strongly resemble a Western Slavic language community.[21]

See also

References

  1. 1 2 Poloni, Estela S; Semino O, Passarino G, Santachiara-Benerecetti AS, Dupanloup I, Langaney A, Excoffier L (1997). "Human genetic affinities for Y-chromosome p49a,f/TaqI haplotypes show strong correspondence with linguistics". American Journal of Human Genetics. 61 (5): 1015–1035. doi:10.1086/301602. PMID 9346874.
  2. Barbujani, Guido (1997). "Invited Editorial: DNA Variation and Language Affinities". American Journal of Human Genetics. 61: 1011–1014. doi:10.1086/301620.
  3. van Driem, George (2010). "The Shompen of Great Nicobar Island: New linguistic and genetic data, and the Austroasiatic homeland revisited". In Keralaputra Shreevinasaiah Nagaraja and Kashyap Mankodi. Austroasiatic Linguistics: Proceedings of the Third International Conference on Austroasiatic Linguistics, 26-28 November 2007. Mysore: Central Institute of Indian Languages. pp. 224–259.
  4. 1 2 3 4 Forster, Peter; Renfrew C (2011). "Mother Tongue and Y Chromosomes". Science. 333 (6048): 1390–1391. doi:10.1126/science.1205331. PMID 21903800.
  5. Ferris, SD; Brown WM, Davidson WS, Wilson AC (1981). "Extensive polymorphism in the mitochondrial DNA of apes". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA. 78 (10): 6319–6323. doi:10.1073/pnas.78.10.6319. PMC 349030Freely accessible. PMID 6273863.
  6. Underhill, Peter; Jin L, Lin AA, Mehdi SQ, Jenkins T, Vollrath D, Davis RW, Cavalli-Sforza LL, Oefner PJ (1997). "Detection of numerous Y chromosome biallelic polymorphisms by denaturing high-performance liquid chromatography". Genome Research. 7 (10): 996–1005. PMC 310671Freely accessible. PMID 9331370.
  7. Hirszfeld, L; Hirszfeld H (1919). "Essai d'application des méthodes sérologiques au probléme des races". Anthropologie. 29: 505–537.
  8. Cavalli-Sforza, Luigi Luca; Menozzi P, Piazza A (1994). The history and Geography of Human Genes. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press.
  9. Darlington, CD (1947). "The genetic component of language". Heredity. 1 (3): 269–286. doi:10.1038/hdy.1947.18.
  10. Cavalli-Sforza, LL; Piazza A, Menozzi P, Mountain J (1988). "Reconstruction of human evolution: bringing together genetic, archaeological, and linguistic data". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA. 85 (16): 6002–6006. doi:10.1073/pnas.85.16.6002. PMC 281893Freely accessible. PMID 3166138.
  11. Poloni, Estella Simone; et al. (2000). "Languages and genes: Modes of transmission observed through the analysis of male-specific and female-specific genes". In Jean-Louis Dessalles and Laleh Ghadakpour. Proceedings: Evolution of Language, 3rd International Conference 3-6 April 2000. Paris: École Nationale Supérieure des Télécommunications. pp. 185–186.
  12. 1 2 3 4 van Driem, George (2007). "Austroasiatic phylogeny and the Austroasiatic homeland in light of recent population genetic studies". Mon-Khmer Studies. 37: 1–14.
  13. van Driem, George (2012). "The ethnolinguistic identity of the domesticators of Asian rice". Comptes Rendus Palevol. 11 (2): 117–132. doi:10.1016/j.crpv.2011.07.004.
  14. 1 2 Zerjal, Tatiana; Pandya A, Santos FR, Adhikari R, Tarazona E, Kayser M, Evgrafov O, Singh L, Thangaraj K, Destro-Bisol G, Thomas MG, Qamar R, Mehdi SQ, Rosser ZH, Hurles ME, Jobling MA, Tyler-Smith C (1999). "The use of Y-chromosomal DNA variation to investigate population history: Recent male spread in Asia and Europe". In Surinder S Papiha, Ranjan Deka and Ranajit Chakraborty. Genomic Diversity. Applications in Human Population Genetic Studies. Williamsburg, Virginia 26 July - 1 Aug 1998. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. pp. 91–101.
  15. 1 2 Chaubey, Gyaneshwer; et al. (2010). "Population genetic structure in Indian Austroasiatic speakers: The role of landscape barriers and sex-specific admixture". Molecular Biology and Evolution. 28 (2): 1013–1024. doi:10.1093/molbev/msq288.
  16. Rai, Niraj; et al. (2012). "The phylogeography of Y-chromosome haplogroup H1a1a-M82 reveals the likely Indian origin of the European Romani populations". PLOS ONE. 7 (11): e48477. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0048477.
  17. 1 2 3 4 van Driem, George (2012). "Etyma, shouldered adzes and molecular variants". In Andrea Ender, Adrian Leemann and Bernhard Wälchli. Methods in Contemporary Linguistics. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
  18. Wen, Bo; et al. (2004). "Genetic evidence supports demic diffusion of Han culture". Nature. 431 (7006): 302–305. doi:10.1038/nature02878. PMID 15372031.
  19. Wood, Elizabeth T; et al. (2005). "Contrasting patterns of Y chromosome and mtDNA variation in Africa: evidence for sex-biased demographic processes". European Journal of Human Genetics. 13 (7): 867–876. doi:10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201408. PMID 15856073.
  20. de Filippo, Cesare; et al. (2011). "Y-Chromosomal Variation in Sub-Saharan Africa: Insights Into the History of Niger-Congo Groups". Molecular Biology and Evolution. 28 (3): 1255–1269. doi:10.1093/molbev/msq312. PMC 3561512Freely accessible. PMID 21109585.
  21. 1 2 3 van Driem, George (2008). "Reflections on the ethnolinguistic prehistory of the greater Himalayan region". In Brigitte Huber, Marianne Volkart and Paul Widmer. Chomolangma, Demawend und Kasbek: Festschrift für Roland Bielmeier zu seinem 65. Geburtstag (2 vols.). Halle: International Institute for Tibetan and Buddhist Studies. pp. 39–59.
  22. Bakker, Pieter Jan (1992). A Language of Our Own’, the Genesis of Michif, the Mixed Cree-French Language of the Canadian Metis. Amsterdam: Universiteit van Amsterdam: doctoral dissertation.
  23. Bakker, Pieter Jan (1994). "Michif, the Cree-French mixed language of the Metis buffalo hunters in Canada". In Peter Bakker and Maarten Mous. Mixed Languages, 15 Case Studies in Language Intertwining (Studies in Language and Language Use, 13). Amsterdam: Instituut voor Fundamenteel Onderzoek naar Taal en Taalgebruik. pp. 13–33.
  24. van Driem, George (2001). Languages of the Himalayas. Leiden: Brill.
  25. Shaywitz, B. A; et al. (1995). "Sex differences in the functional organization of the brain for language". Nature. 373 (6515): 607–609. doi:10.1038/373607a0. PMID 7854416.
  26. Pugh, K. R; et al. (1996). "Cerebral organization of component processes in reading". Brain. 119: 1221–1238. doi:10.1093/brain/119.4.1221.
  27. Pugh, K. R; et al. (1997). "Predicting reading performance from neuroimaging profiles: The cerebral basis of phonological effects in printed word identification". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance. 23: 299–318. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.23.2.299.
  28. Jaeger; et al. (1998). "Sex differences in brain regions activated by grammatical and reading tasks". NeuroReport. 9: 2803–2807. doi:10.1097/00001756-199808240-00022.
  29. Kansaku, K; et al. (2000). "Sex differences in lateralization revealed in the posterior language areas". Cerebral Cortex. 10 (9): 866–872. doi:10.1093/cercor/10.9.866. PMID 10982747.
  30. Rossell, S. L; et al. (2002). "Sex differences in functional brain activation during a lexical visual field task". Brain & Language. 80: 97–105. doi:10.1006/brln.2000.2449.
  31. Baxter, L. C; et al. (2003). "Sex differences in semantic language processing: A functional MRI study". Brain & Language. 84: 264–272. doi:10.1016/s0093-934x(02)00549-7.
  32. Clements, A. M; et al. (2006). "Sex differences in cerebral laterality of language and visuospatial processing". Brain & Language. 98: 150–158. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2006.04.007.
  33. Burman, Douglas D.; et al. (2008). "Sex differences in neural processing of language among children". Neuropsychologia. 46: 1349–1362. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.021.
This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the 6/3/2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.