Surround suppression

Surround suppression is an effect seen when neurons do not respond as much when the stimulus is enlarged. It is typically observed in electrophysiology studies of the brain and has been noted in many sensory neurons, most notably in the early visual system. Surround suppression is defined as a reduction in the activity of a neuron in response to a stimulus outside its classical receptive field. This nonlinear effect is one of many that reveals the complexity of biological sensory systems, and the connections of properties of neurons that may cause this effect are still being studied. The characteristics, mechanisms, and perceptual consequences of this phenomenon are of interest to many communities, including neurobiology, computational neuroscience, psychology, and computer vision.

Background

Stimulation of the surround with the classical receptive field induces a response that is smaller than stimulation of the classical receptive field alone. Stimulation of the surround only has no effect.

The classical model of early vision presumes that each neuron responds independently to a specific stimulus in a localized area of the visual field. The stimulus and corresponding location in the visual field are collectively called the classical receptive field, which often resembles a Gabor filter. However, not all effects can be explained by this idea of independent filters. Surround suppression is one of several effects in which neurons do not behave according to the classical model. These effects are collectively called non-classical receptive field effects, and have recently become a substantial research area in vision and other sensory systems.

During surround suppression, neurons are inhibited by a stimulus outside their classical receptive field, in an area deemed the 'surround.'

Characteristics: effects on neural responses

Electrophysiology studies

Electrophysiology studies are used to characterize the surround suppression effect. Vision researchers that record neural activity in the primary visual cortex (V1) have seen that spike rates, or neural responses, can be suppressed in as many as 90% of neurons[1][2] by stimuli outside of their surround. In these cells, the spike rates are reduced by as much as 70%.[3]

Stimulus and attention dependence

Suppression often increases with stimulus size beyond the classical receptive field.

The suppressive effect is often dependent on the contrast, orientation, and direction of motion of the stimulus stimulating the surround. These properties are highly dependent on the brain area and the individual neuron being studied. In MT, for instance, cells can be sensitive to the direction and velocity of stimuli up to 50 to 100 times the area of their classical receptive fields.[4] The statistical properties of the stimuli used to probe these neurons affect the properties of the surround as well. Because these areas are so highly interconnected, stimulation of one cell can affect the response properties of other cells, and therefore researchers have become increasingly aware of the choice of stimuli they use in these experiments. In addition to studies with simple stimuli (dots, bars, sinusoidal gratings),[3][5][6] more recent studies have used more realistic stimuli (natural scenes) to study these effects.[7] Stimuli that better represent natural scenes tend to induce higher levels of suppression, indicating this effect is tied closely to the properties of natural scenes such as textures and local context.

Surround suppression is also modulated by attention. By training monkeys to attend to certain areas of their visual field, researchers have studied how directed attention can enhance the suppressive effects of stimuli surrounding the area of attention.[8] Similar perceptual studies have been performed on human subjects as well.

Systems involved

Surround suppression was formally discovered in the visual pathway, and noticed first by Hubel and Wiesel[5] while mapping receptive fields. The earliest parts of the visual pathway: the retina, Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN), and primary visual cortex (V1) are among the most well-studied. Surround suppression has been studied in later areas as well, including V2, V3, V4,[2] and MT.[9]

Surround suppression has also been seen in sensory systems other than vision. One example in somatosensation is surround suppression in the barrel cortex of mice, in which bending one whisker can suppress the response of a neuron responding to a whisker nearby.[10] It has even been seen in the frequency response properties of electoreception in electric fish.[11]

Biological mechanisms

The biological mechanisms behind surround suppression have remained a point of contention.[10]

The differences between lateral, feedforward, and recurrent connections

Several theories have been proposed for the biological basis of this effect. Based on the diversity of the stimulus characteristics that cause this effect and the variety of responses that are generated, it seems that many mechanisms may be at play.

Lateral connections

Lateral connections are connections between neurons in the same layer. There are many of these connections in all areas of the visual system, which means that a neuron representing one piece of the visual field can influence a neuron representing another piece. Even within lateral connections, there are potentially different mechanisms at play. Monocular mechanisms, requiring stimulation in only one eye, may drive this effect with stimuli with high spatial frequency. When the stimulus frequency is lowered, however, binocular mechanisms come into play, where neurons from different eyes may suppress each other.[12] Model based on this idea have been shown to reproduce surround suppressive effects.

Recurrent feedback

It has been posited that lateral connections are too slow and cover too little of the visual field to fully explain surround suppression.[13] Feedback from higher areas may explain the discrepancies seen in mechanism for surround suppression based purely on lateral connections. There is evidence that inactivation of higher order areas results in reduced strength of surround suppression.[13] At least one model of excitatory connections from higher levels has been formed in the effort to more fully explain surround suppression.[14] However, recurrent feedback is difficult to determine using electrophysiology, and the potential mechanisms at play are not as well studied as feedforward or lateral connections.

Advantages

Surround suppression behavior gives the sensory system several advantages from both a perceptual and information theory standpoint.

Perceptual advantages

Surround suppression likely participates in context-dependent perceptual tasks. Some specific tasks in which surround suppression may aid include:

These tasks require the use of inputs over wide regions of visual space, meaning that independent responses to small parts of the visual field (a classical linear model of V1) would not be able to produce these effects. There is evidence that surround suppression participates in these tasks by either adjusting the representation of the classical receptive field or representing entirely different features that include both the classical receptive field and the surround. Direct comparison between physiology and psychophysical experiments have been done on several perceptual effects. These include: (1) the reduced apparent contrast of a grating texture embedded in a surrounding grating, (2) target identification when flanked by other features, (3) saliency of broken contours surrounded by edge segments of different orientations, and (4) orientation discrimination when surrounded by features of different orientations and spatial frequencies.[19]

Information theoretic advantages

It has recently been shown that stimulation of the surround may support the efficient coding hypothesis proposed by Horace Barlow in 1961.[20] This hypothesis suggests that the goal of the sensory system is to create an efficient representation of the stimulus. Recently, this has intersected with the idea of a 'sparse' code, one that is represented using the fewest units possible. It has been shown that surround suppression increases the efficiency of transmitting visual information, and may form a sparse code.[21] If many cells respond to parts of the same stimulus, for instance, a lot of redundant information is encoded.[22] The cell needs metabolic energy for each action potential it produces. Therefore, surround suppression likely helps to produce a neural code that is more metabolically efficient. There are additional theoretical advantages, including the removal of statistical redundancy inherent in natural scene statistics, as well as decorrelation of neural responses,[7] which means less information to process later in the pathway.

Related approaches in computer vision

Edge detection based on on center (red) and off center (green) receptive fields

The goal of computer vision is to perform automated tasks similar to those of the human visual system, quickly and accurately interpreting the world and making decisions based on visual information. Because surround suppression seems to play a role in efficient and accurate perception, there have been a few computer vision algorithms inspired by this phenomenon in human vision:

So far, the scientific community has been focused on the response properties of the neurons, but exploration of the relation to inference and learning has begun as well.[25]

References

  1. Cavanaugh, JR; Bair, W; Movshon, JA (2002a). "Nature and interaction of signals from the receptive field center and surround in macaque V1 neurons". J Neurophysiol. 88: 2530–2546. doi:10.1152/jn.00692.2001.
  2. 1 2 3 4 Allman, J; Miezin, F; McGuinness, E (1985). "Stimulus specific responses from beyond the classical receptive field : Neurophysiological mechanisms for local-global comparisons in visual neurons". Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 8: 407–30. doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.08.030185.002203.
  3. 1 2 3 Jones, H. E.; Grieve, K. L.; Wang, W.; Sillito, A. M. (2001). "Surround Suppression in Primate V1 Surround Suppression in Primate V1". J Neurophysiol. 86: 2011–2028.
  4. Allman, J; Miezin, F; McGuinness, E (1985). "Direction- and velocity-specific responses from beyond the classical receptive field in the middle temporal visual area (MT)". Perception. 14 (2): 105–126. doi:10.1068/p140105.
  5. 1 2 Hubel, D. H.; Wiesel, T. N. (1965). "RECEPTIVE FIELDS AND FUNCTIONAL ARCHITECTURE IN TWO NONSTRIATE VISUAL AREAS (18 AND 19) OF THE CAT". Journal of Neurophysiology. 28: 229–89. PMID 14283058.
  6. Ozeki, H.; Finn, I. M.; Schaffer, E. S.; Miller, K. D.; Ferster, D. (2009). "Inhibitory stabilization of the cortical network underlies visual surround suppression". Neuron. 62 (4): 578–592. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.028.
  7. 1 2 Haider, B.; Krause, M. R.; Duque, A.; Yu, Y.; Touryan, J.; Mazer; McCormick (2010). "Synaptic and network mechanisms of sparse and reliable visual cortical activity during nonclassical receptive field stimulation". Neuron. 65 (1): 107–21. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2009.12.005.
  8. Sundberg, Mitchell; Reynolds (2009). "Spatial Attention Modulates Center-Surround Interactions in Macaque Visual Area V4". Neuron. 61 (6): 952–963. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2009.02.023.
  9. Hunter, J. N.; Born, R. T. (2011). "Stimulus-dependent modulation of suppressive influences in MT.". The Journal of Neuroscience. 31 (2): 678–86. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4560-10.2011.
  10. 1 2 Sachdev, R. N. S.; Krause, M. R.; Mazer, J. A. (2012). "Surround suppression and sparse coding in visual and barrel cortices". Frontiers in neural circuits. 6: 43. doi:10.3389/fncir.2012.00043.
  11. Chacron, M. J.; Doiron, B.; Maler, L.; Longtin, A.; Bastian, J. (2003). "Non-classical receptive field mediates switch in a sensory neuron's frequency tuning". Nature. 423 (6935): 77–81. doi:10.1038/nature01590.
  12. Webb, B. S.; Dhruv, N. T.; Solomon, S. G.; Tailby, C.; Lennie, P. (2005). "Early and late mechanisms of surround suppression in striate cortex of macaque". The Journal of Neuroscience. 25 (50): 11666–75. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3414-05.2005.
  13. 1 2 Angelucci, A.; Bullier, J. (2003). "Reaching beyond the classical receptive field of V1 neurons: horizontal or feedback axons?". Journal of physiology, Paris. 97 (2-3): 141–54. doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2003.09.001.
  14. Sullivan, T. J.; de Sa, V. R. (2006). "A model of surround suppression through cortical feedback". Neural networks. 19 (5): 564–72. doi:10.1016/j.neunet.2005.12.003.
  15. Perrone, J. A. (2012). "A neural-based code for computing image velocity from small sets of middle temporal (MT/V5) neuron inputs". Journal of vision. 12: 8. doi:10.1167/12.8.1.
  16. Hess, R.; Field, D. (1999). "Integration of contours: new insights". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 3 (12): 480–486. doi:10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01410-2. PMID 10562727.
  17. Field, D. J.; Hayes, A.; Hess, R. F. (1993). "Contour integration by the human visual system: evidence for a local "association field". Vision Research. 33 (2): 173–93. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(93)90156-q. PMID 8447091.
  18. Supèr, H; Romeo, A; Keil, M (2010). "Feed-Forward Segmentation of Figure-Ground and Assignment of Border-Ownership". PLoS ONE. 5 (5): e10705. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0010705.
  19. Seriès, P.; Lorenceau, J.; Frégnac, Y. (2004). "The "silent" surround of V1 receptive fields: theory and experiments". Journal of physiology, Paris. 97 (4-6): 453–74. doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2004.01.023.
  20. Barlow, H. B. (1961). Possible principles underlying the transformation of sensory messages. Sensory communication, 217-234.
  21. Vinje, W. E.; Gallant, J. L. (2002). "Natural stimulation of the nonclassical receptive field increases information transmission efficiency in V1". The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 22 (7): 2904–15.
  22. Schwartz, O., & Simoncelli, E. (2001). Natural signal statistics and sensory gain control. Nature neuroscience 819–825. Retrieved from http://www.cns.nyu.edu/csh04/Articles/Schwartz-Simoncelli-01.pdf.
  23. Wei, H.; Wang, Z.; Zuo, Q. (2012). "A Model of Image Representation Based on Non-classical Receptive Fields Neural Mechanism of Non-classical Receptive Field". LNCS. 7368: 297–306.
  24. Grigorescu, C.; Petkov, N.; Westenberg (2003). "Contour detection based on nonclassical receptive field inhibition". IEEE transactions on image processing. 12 (7): 729–39. doi:10.1109/TIP.2003.814250.
  25. Hyvärinen, A (2010). "Statistical Models of Natural Images and Cortical Visual Representation". Topics in Cognitive Science. 2 (2): 251–264. doi:10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01057.
This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the 6/28/2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.